On cuteness: unlocking the parental brain and beyond (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

On cuteness: unlocking the parental brain and beyond (1)

Link to Publisher's site

Trends Cogn Sci. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2017 Jul 1.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC4956347

NIHMSID: NIHMS788929

PMID: 27211583

Author information Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at Trends Cogn Sci

Abstract

Cuteness in offspring is a potent protective mechanism that ensures survival for otherwise completely dependent infants. Previous research has linked cuteness to early ethological ideas of a “kindchenschema” (infant schema) where infant facial features serve as “innate releasing mechanisms” for instinctual caregiving behaviours. We propose extending the concept of cuteness beyond visual features to include positive infant sounds and smells. Evidence from behavioural and neuroimaging studies links this extended concept of cuteness to simple “instinctual” behaviours and to caregiving, protection and complex emotions. We review how cuteness supports key parental capacities by igniting fast privileged neural activity followed by slower processing in large brain networks also involved in play, empathy, and perhaps even higher-order moral emotions.

Keywords: Caregiving, affiliative, neuroimaging, emotion, pleasure, kawaii

Cuteness for caregiving, empathy, and beyond

What is it about the sight of an infant that makes almost everyone crack a smile? Big eyes, chubby cheeks, and a button nose? An infectious laugh, soft skin, and a captivating smell? These characteristics contribute to “cuteness” and propel our caregiving behaviours, which is vital as infants need our constant attention to survive and thrive. Infants attract us through all our senses, which helps make cuteness one of the most basic and powerful forces shaping our behaviour.

The prevailing view of cuteness came from the founding fathers of ethology, Nobel prizewinners Konrad Lorenz and Niko Tinbergen. They proposed that the cute facial features of infants form a “kindchenschema” (infant schema), a prime example of an “innate releasing mechanism” that unlocks instinctual behaviours [1]. This hypothesis was part of their larger ethological program to define the biological study of behaviour. The program included at least four goals, namely, to define the physiology, survival value, evolution, and development of behaviour [2, 3]. These goals are still relevant today, but subsequent research has questioned some of their initial propositions, such as “innate releasers”—and the whole idea of “instincts” [4-6]. The tools of modern neuroscience have provided the basis for a broader understanding and precise dissection of brain networks that process survival-related stimuli [7] and have clarified the impact of biologically relevant stimuli on brain networks related to motivation, pleasure and learning [8].

Here, we propose to extend the concept of cuteness beyond the morphological features of the infant face to include positive auditory and olfactory features that attract parental caregiving. Infant laughs and babbles are examples of what has been called auditory cuteness [9], where the infant's well-stretched vibrating vocal membrane produces mostly high frequency and pure tone-like sounds that attract caregivers across many bird and mammalian species [10]. Most existing neuroscientific research has not defined such auditory features as cute, but here we synthesize the existing research on stimuli belonging to our extended notion of cuteness and propose that impact of cuteness on emotions and behavior is much broader than suggested by the idea of “instincts”. Cuteness is linked to the helplessness of human infants as a key (but not sole) elicitor of complex parental caregiving [11]. It works on both fast and slow time scales; it elicits core affective brain activity through fast attentional biasing and slow appraisal processes. Our synthesis also indicates that there is a need to rethink the role of cuteness: It is a powerful positive stimulus and even non-infant stimuli can become cute. Beyond caregiving, cuteness appears to play a key role in facilitating social relationships, pleasure, and well-being. As such, we speculate that cuteness may even go beyond eliciting caregiving to facilitate complex social relationships by triggering empathy and compassion [12].

In what follows, we first discuss the existing behavioural and neuroimaging evidence for the fast processing of infant and infant-like cute stimuli. We investigate how our extended concept of cuteness helps to unlock complex caregiving even in adults who are not parents. This caregiving cannot be reduced to mere instinctual behaviour, but instead requires ‘expertise’ that takes time to acquire, and this slow acquisition changes the caregiver's brain. We discuss the implications of how problems for parents, such as postnatal (postpartum) depression, and for infants, such as cleft-lip, alter the processing of infant stimuli and disrupt natural caregiving. We present evidence suggesting that cuteness can also facilitate slow, complex behaviours also involved in caregiving. We speculate that the modulatory effect of cuteness on brain networks could be linked to mechanisms for privileged access to consciousness. As such we hazard that cuteness might usefully be construed as a potential candidate for expanding the “moral circle” {Singer, 1981 #5627} of entities worthy of moral consideration to those outside the circle by increasing empathy and compassion.

Fast responses to infants

Cute infants attract our attention, and they also capture it quickly. Here we extend the concept of infant cuteness to be a biologically significant, positive multimodal stimulus that through sight, sound, or smell, elicits fast selective attentional processing that facilitates caregiving and other complex emotional behaviours. Other biologically significant negative stimuli, such as the infant cry, also elicit fast selective attentional processing [13-15]. These abilities enable infants to quickly affect people's—both parents and nonparents—brains and minds, which opens up the possibility for complex caregiving and the promotion of sociality [1, 16]. Cuteness, then, displays both instantaneous impacts and gradual effects that aid infants’ evolutionary aim of survival, perhaps linked to both proximate and ultimate evolutionary functions [17].

Behavioural data demonstrate the salience and attentional prioritisation of infant cues, such as a cute face, on which most of research has concentrated. The visual features that make infant faces cute include large, round eyes, a head “too large” for the body, high eyebrows, full cheeks, and a small chin [1, 18] (Figure 1A). Adults prefer to look at cuter infant faces [19-21] and even prefer them to adult faces [14, 22]. The impact of cuteness transcends in-group versus out-group distinctions and cultural familiarity [23]. Infants and children also prefer to look at cuter infant faces [24, 25]. Cuteness’ power to capture attention may diminish as a child develops—both adults and children pay more attention to infant's faces than older children's faces [26], suggesting that the power of cuteness in young children's faces fades as children mature. Crucially, cuteness does not operate alone, and may be influenced by experience. This influence is easily demonstrated in a simple computerised “baby social reward task” where learning about an infant's easy or difficult temperament shifts subsequent cuteness ratings [27].

On cuteness: unlocking the parental brain and beyond (2)

Behavioural measures of cuteness of faces across species

A) The proportions of features of a face can be used to provide objective measurements of cuteness in infant and adults [20]. B) Adult men and women (who are not yet parents) differ in their liking ratings but not in the amount of effort they expend on viewing natural images of infants with varying levels of objective cuteness [22]. C) Artificially changing the proportions of the faces of humans, dogs and cats can change their perceived cuteness, although questions have been raised over the ecological validity of such nonecological image manipulations. D) Five year old children find the young significantly cuter than the adult of different species [24].

Infant cues spur us to action: both men and women will expend extra effort to look longer at cute infant faces [22, 28] (For putative sex differences, see Box 1.) When presented with cute and less-cute infants, adults prefer to give a toy to or even adopt the cuter one [29]. Adults who see an infant face prior to a simple task have faster reaction times and sustain their engagement in the task [30].

Cuteness is therefore a fundamental mechanism that helps to elicit caregiving. As mentioned above, cuteness is not is not limited to visual cues but works through the other senses: infant laughter and babbling [31, 32], and even smell [33]. When we have oriented to and recognised an infant—which cuteness helps to secure—slower and more complex behaviours can begin, starting with ‘intuitive parenting’ [34], and leading to higher-order capacities, such as intersubjectivity, attachment, and cognitive functions [35].

It is interesting to compare the positive, cute infant stimulus to the negative, aversive stimulus of, for example, an infant distress cry. Analogous to the effect of seeing an infant face or hearing infant laughter, adults also increase their effort in simple motor tasks after hearing the “biological siren” of an infant cry [36]. Both positive and negative infant cues provide evidence that they take a ‘fast’ pathway in the brain. Our brains are equipped to respond quickly to a baby cry, whether we are female or male, parents or non-parents. That is, even nulliparous adults display very fast brain responses to both auditory [37, 38] and visual [15, 39] infant cues. Important general differences between negative aversive stimuli, such as crying, and positive inviting stimuli, such as cute babbling and laughter, are found in the parental behaviour that follows them. Whereas crying initiates less flexible, more stereotyped behaviour to prevent or terminate the noise, cuteness promotes sociality, smiling, laughter, and more complex interactions designed to continue the interaction [40]. Cuteness clearly facilitates caregiving, and the cuteness response is not limited to instincts, but is characterised by complex interactions.

Neuroimaging data provide evidence of rapid, intuitive responses to infant cues followed later by processing in several brain regions [15, 39, 41]. Magnetoencephalography (MEG) has shown that men and women, parents and non-parents all have rapid—within 140 ms—selective neural responses to infant faces [15] (Figure 2A). This rapid activity is found in the orbitofrontal cortex when participants look at infant faces, but not adult faces or infant faces with a craniofacial abnormality that disrupts the typical cute facial composition [39]. The orbitofrontal cortex has been implicated in representing salient stimuli at multiple time-scales [42-45] possibly to coordinate the brain's resources for further action and sociality [46]. Experience also plays a role here, as parents’ brains respond to their own, rather than to unfamiliar, infants with stronger activity in reward-related regions [47-49].

On cuteness: unlocking the parental brain and beyond (3)

Cuteness elicits fast responses in the human brain

A) Infant faces are examples of cute stimuli which have been shown to elicit fast brain responses in the orbitofrontal cortex (OFC, ~130ms) at the same time as responses in the fusiform face region [15]. B) Artificially manipulating the cuteness of infant faces have been shown to correlate with changes in the BOLD signal in the nucleus accumbens, part of the pleasure system [41]. C) Very fast neural responses (~50ms) are found in the human brainstem to both positive and negative infant vocalisations (babbling, laughter and crying) [37]. D) Similar to the fast brain response to cute visual stimuli, infant crying elicits activity in the OFC (~140ms) at the same time as activity in primary sensory cortices [58].

Of course, other stimuli can be cute; we raise animals, such as puppies and kittens, to look cute (see Figure 1C). Children and adults have given similar cuteness ratings to animal and infant pictures [24, 50]. Adults find young cats and dogs cuter than adult cats and dogs, although pet owners show higher cuteness ratings for human infants, suggesting that expertise plays a role [51]. Cartoon characters like Mickey Mouse have become ‘cuter’ and more baby-like over time [52]. Cuteness has been transferred to inanimate objects, such as cars and toys [53]. The iconic children's toy, the teddy bear, has gradually changed to become more baby-like, which may have evolved through a process of artificial selection whereby consumers chose ever-cuter bears [53].

Similarly, the Japanese term “Kawaii” is often translated into English as cute, although its etymological roots are in the ancient word kawa-hayu-shi that literally means face (kawa-) flushing (hayu-shi) [23, 40], as demonstrated in an experiment using thermography to show an increase of facial temperature in adults when viewing infant faces [23]. Kawaii objects share many of the cute-infant-schema features and viewing kawaii images has also demonstrated behavioural task improvements and a narrowed attentional focus [40].

Facilitation of parental capacities for caregiving

Empirical evidence suggests that cuteness plays a key role in facilitating the parent-infant relationship, which is a highly dynamic and intensely social template of all later human relationships. For infants this dynamic starts with basic orienting and recognition processes and culminates in attaining higher socio-emotional and cognitive capacities [35, 54, 55]. This slow process is shaped by social interactions with primary caregivers, typically parents, who in turn rely on infant signals to guide their interactions [56, 57]. Becoming a parent can be daunting at first, but parent-infant interactions are full of reciprocal influences and each party comes to the task well equipped. Just as infants have excellent competences, such as cuteness and crying, that elicit attention and care, parents have capacities that facilitate optimal care.

To be able to provide this care, at least three “parental capacities” have to develop: 1) a focus of attention on the infant and an associated contingent responsiveness; 2) emotional scaffolding especially when the infant is distressed; and 3) behavioural sensitivity to attachment cues and mentalization (the capacity to treat an infant as a psychological agent) [46]. The antecedents to these capacities, particularly attentional focus, are found even in the brain processes of non-parents [15, 58].

A key question for the immediate future of work in this area is how parental ‘expertise’ develops and whether our perceptions of cuteness change over time or with experience. Despite the fundamental capacity to orient and respond to infants, their care requires a complex and demanding array of skills (e.g., emotional scaffolding and mentalization) that parents must acquire and hone. There is a wealth of longitudinal, cross-cultural behavioural data on parenting [55, 59], but a dearth of neuroimaging data on longitudinal changes in the parental brain. Except from two recent studies which investigated structural changes in gray matter in parents at two timepoints post-partum [60, 61], all previous neuroimaging work has been cross-sectional studies of parents and non-parents [46, 57]. We are, however, in the process of carrying out the first longitudinal functional and structural neuroimaging study of the parental brain before conception, immediately after birth and 12 months later.

What has emerged from the cross-sectional studies of the parental brain is the engagement of brain networks known to play roles in emotion, pleasure, social interactions, mentalization, and embodied simulation [57, 62, 63]. Important hubs in these emotional networks include the orbitofrontal cortex, anterior cingulate cortex, anterior insula, amygdala, and supplementary motor area [64]. As shown earlier, the orbitofrontal cortex in particular appears to be crucial for recognizing infant cues as salient, tagging them for fast processing, and orchestrating much slower subsequent appraisal behaviours [46]. These findings have led to the hypothesis that the orbitofrontal cortex is a key brain region that changes as parental expertise develops.

None of these brain networks appears to be unique to caregiving behaviour. Although they are recruited by both positive and negative infant-related stimuli, these networks are also recruited in other prosocial contexts. The evolutionary importance of infant survival means that infant-related stimuli are extremely salient and able to sustain metabolically expensive behaviours, such as parental caregiving, over longer time-scales. Both negative and positive infant stimuli are important for the fast responses involved in caregiving [14, 37]. Negative stimuli, such as crying, have been intensively investigated for their role in rapidly instigating and sustaining caregiving [37, 58, 65]. Positive stimuli, such as cute smiling infant faces and babbling, are equally able to incite fast brain responses [14, 15, 37]. There is some evidence of kinship-related cuteness, i.e. of differences in parents’ prioritization of own versus other's infants in terms of attentional [66] and emotional [67, 68] processing. But cute stimuli in general also engage other slower prosocial behaviours, such as play and empathy [12, 69].

Difficulties in prioritising attentional resporces to cute stimuli

Neuroscientific research is helping to explicate parental and infant capacities, which is important because difficulties in the parent-infant relationship present a major challenge, as early childhood often proves crucial for long-term well-being [54]. A better understanding of problems in the parent-infant relationship affords unique opportunities to invest in prevention—an exceedingly efficient societal strategy [70].

Disruption in parental sensitivity to infant cues is commonly found in parental postnatal (also known as postpartum) depression (PND) [e.g., 76], which affects substantial numbers of both mothers and fathers, typically 10-15% in high-income countries [71, 72] and up to 30% of mothers in low- and middle-income countries [73]. PND can affect parenting in many different ways. It has been associated with altered parent-infant interactions [74, 75], including, as mentioned, disruptions in parental sensitivity to infant cues [e.g., 76]. It particularly can affect cognitive processes, such as attention and motivation, that are vital for developing parenting capabilities. In terms of responsiveness to infant signals, for example, both mothers with PND [77, 78] and adults with depression [38, 79] exhibit disrupted sensitivity to negative stimuli, such as distress in infant cries and faces. PND has also been associated with increased risk for childhood cognitive and socio-emotional problems [80, 81].

Changes in infants’ appearance and signals can have significant long-term adverse effects on their development because such changes compromise cuteness. The most studied example is cleft lip and palate, probably because it is one of the most common congenital conditions, occurring in 1 out of 700 live births in the UK [82]. Having cleft lip changes the typical “cute” infant facial configuration (Figure 3) even though only a small proportion of the face is affected. This morphological change, in turn, is associated with adverse outcomes in child development, including cognitive problems. These problems can at least partly be attributed to early disruptions in mother-child interactions—specifically a lack of all-important maternal responsiveness [83]. Non-parents report finding infants with cleft lip less “cute” than typical infants [84], and when viewing cleft-lip infant faces, early activity in the orbitofrontal cortex is significantly diminished compared to viewing typical infant faces [39, 84].

On cuteness: unlocking the parental brain and beyond (4)

Even minor facial abnormalities can change cuteness perception

A-D) Behavioural findings to images of human and animal with cleft lips show significantly stronger liking and wanting of non-cleft stimuli [84]. E) Neuroimaging findings of significantly diminished fast processing (<140ms) in OFC of cleft infant faces compared to cute infant faces in nonparents [39].

Developing effective interventions has traditionally relied on careful observations of behaviour, but is soon likely to be bolstered by a better understanding of the fundamental brain mechanisms associated with caregiving and how brain processing can be disrupted. Improving our understanding of the brain processes related to positive and negative infant signals could create opportunities for improving interventions. New learning paradigms derived from neuroscientific findings have already shown considerable promise [27, 79]. For example, the “baby-social-reward-task”, which uses cute sounds and faces, mimics important aspects of caregiving and has been able to shift perceptions of infant temperament [27, 85]. Musical training has also been implicated in sensitivity and empathy for infant distress [79, 86], in that specific training has improved caregiver sensitivity. Such targeted behavioural paradigms based on neuroscientific research may eventually help to increase caregivers’ ability to properly interpret infant signals and provide appropriate responses.

Putative brain mechanisms of cuteness

Cuteness has to be seen in the light of the evolutionary imperatives implying that for stimuli relevant to the survival of individuals and species (food, sex, progeny) to easily gain access to consciousness, they must be prioritized over other, less salient stimuli. Affective neuroscience has shown how positive and negative survival-related stimuli selectively capture attention, elicit core affects, and are available for conscious appraisal [87, 88]. A large body of emotion research has identified brain networks where valenced stimuli are evaluated—based on the current state of the individual—for their reward value and then made available for future decision-making [64]. This processing is supported by the pleasure cycle, which consists of appetitive, consummatory, and satiety phases (Figure 4B). Dissociable brain mechanisms associated with the wanting, liking, and learning of rewards have also been linked to specific brain regions that govern phase transitions within the pleasure cycle (Figure 4A) [89]. The breakdown of any of these mechanisms leads to anhedonia, the lack of pleasure, which is a significant feature in neuropsychiatric disorders [90].

On cuteness: unlocking the parental brain and beyond (5)

Putative brain mechanisms of cuteness and other infant survival-relevant stimuli elicit fast and slow affiliative behaviours

A) The core affect elicited by cuteness is generated by the pleasure network in the human brain (shown here in one hemisphere only) with the nucleus accumbens and ventral pallidum (in red) and other main pleasure coding regions (in green). Clockwise views (from bottom left) are from top, front, side and 3D perspective. The connections indicate the tentative functional networks mediating hedonic “liking” reactions and subjective pleasure ratings [64]. B) The optimization of resource allocation for survival depends on the engagement with rewards that act as motivational magnets to initiate, sustain and switch state. Typically, the pleasure cycle consists of appetitive, consummation and satiety phases, where wanting, liking and learning mechanisms drive the phase transitions of the metastable brain states [102]. C) Subliminally presented stimuli and non-survival related stimuli often fail to provide ignition of activity that is made available for global access [103]. D) Cuteness and other infant survival-relevant stimuli provide privileged routes to fast ignition of activity leading to glocal access providing the necessary slowness for prosocial caregiving and play behaviours [43, 93, 104]. Abbreviations: Nucleus accumbens (NAc), ventral pallidum (VP), parabrachial nucleus (PBN); medial OFC (mOFC); lateral OFC (lOFC); mid-anterior OFC (midOFC); dorsal anterior cingulate cortex (dACC); rostral anterior cingulate cortex (rACC); and periaqueductal gray (PAG).

In this Review we have argued that cuteness is an important, positive, infant-related signal that elicits both fast and slow brain activity linked to affiliative behaviours and this cuteness is not limited to infants but can be extended to other species and even inanimate objects. Survival-relevant positive cuteness as well as negative stimuli, such as infants’ crying, have been shown to selectively bias our attention through rapid activity in the orbitofrontal cortex which is then followed by slower, sustained processing in wider brain networks associated with emotion appraisal [14, 15, 58]. Importantly, significant progress has been made in understanding the dynamics of functional brain networks [91], and in particular the state-dependent coupling that is fundamental to implementing flexible effective communication between different brain regions—without changing the fixed underlying structure of the brain [92].

In terms of understanding the impact of cuteness-induced activity, the global neuronal workspace model is a possible account for how conscious access to stimuli is made possible by igniting activity in self-supporting, reverberating, metastable networks that broadcast information to the whole brain [93-96]. In this context, we speculate that cuteness (through its elemental, rapid, robust, and pervasive nature) could provide privileged, multimodal access to consciousness through global workspace mechanisms (Figure 4D). Of course, other potential mechanisms should also be evaluated. Further, whole-brain computational modelling (see Box 2) [97] can now be used to identify causal mechanisms of brain networks involved in the segregation and integration of information over the fast and slow timescales involved in caregiving [92, 98].

Cuteness: beyond caregiving to empathy?

Few things have the power to move us to action as the cuteness of infants. This proposition was powerfully demonstrated by the international reaction to the tragic drowning of a cute little three-year-old Syrian refugee in the Mediterranean Sea in September 2015. The published photo caused moral outrage and a groundswell of sympathy for the plight of these refugees. The tragedy led to a surge in donations to charities and seemed to change moral concern for the Syrian refugees. Whereas previously the refugees were often treated with indifference and to a large extent ‘dehumanized’ as an out-group, they suddenly became part of the in-group and granted entry in large numbers (in contrast to, e.g., African boat migrants).

As noted earlier, cuteness is a general promoter of sociality acting through mentalization, the ability to treat infants and even inanimate objects as psychological agents [12]. As such cuteness may more generally serve to maximize moral concern by expanding the moral circle, that is the boundary drawn around entities deemed worthy of moral consideration [99]. This proposal contrasts with disgust which contracts the moral circle through contagion processes that extend disgust to categories beyond food stimuli (e.g., inanimate objects [100]). Out-group members are often seen in this way and dehumanized as a consequence.

Thus, disgust and cuteness would appear to regulate—albeit in opposite directions—the way people mentalize objects that are not already highly mentalized. Given that children are (in principle) universally forbidden targets of harm, cuteness could serve to expand the moral circle. Any cute infant is automatically granted membership to the moral circle and cuteness can then be extended to other people (or objects). As such cuteness-triggered positive mentalizing could instigate wider social engagement and perhaps even empathy and compassion [12].

Concluding remarks

We have proposed to extend the concept of cuteness to be a biologically significant, positive multimodal stimulus that through sight, sound, or smell of infants can help facilitate caregiving and perhaps promote other sophisticated emotional behaviours. We have presented established as well as emerging evidence that as such cuteness is a potent positive stimulus, which can elicit very fast brain responses that prioritise infant signals in women and men, parents and non-parents. Cuteness helps infants to survive by eliciting caregiving, which cannot be reduced to simple, instinctual behaviours. Instead, caregiving involves a complex choreography of slow, careful, deliberate and long-lasting prosocial behaviours, which ignite fundamental brain pleasure systems that are also engaged when eating food or listening to music [89], and always involve pleasant experiences.

We argued here that cuteness goes beyond an attention-grabbing evolutionary strategy infants use to attract care and protection. Instead, like a Trojan horse, cuteness opens doors that might otherwise remain shut. In terms of mechanisms, we proposed that cuteness ignites activity in metastable brain networks, which provides a framework for sustaining the slowness inherent to prosocial behaviours [91]. Cuteness encourages caregiving and carefree playfulness. It is easily extended to other species, such as cats and dogs, and even cute inanimate objects, such as Mickey Mouse, Hello Kitty, and the teddy bears we treasure. Cuteness works through all our senses—so, besides what we see, we are, for example, also drawn to cute babbling and cute melodies [101]. We speculate that the anthropomorphizing that accompanies cuteness might serve to extend our moral circle, and counteract the dehumanization and xenophobia all too common in our nature. Cuteness could be useful in designing novel interventions to strengthen troubled parent-infant relationships. It could also be used to increase sympathy and empathy for those in the in-group and compassion toward those in the out-group. Perhaps cuteness is best thought of as a mirror to our nature and a powerful reminder of our inherent need for simple pleasures.

Box 1: Gender and cuteness

Sexual dimorphism in responsiveness to cute infant cues is an important area of research to understand how cuteness affects us. Yet, a focus upon a monotropy in terms of infant-parent relationships has led to an inequity within the literature, with mothers favoured at the expense of fathers [105]. These ideas may have recourse to the work of the founding father of attachment theory John Bowlby [16], who proposed that children are born with a biological predisposition to form one exclusive attachment relationship. Bowlby attributed this unique relationship to the mother, proposing that a single maternal relationship was enough to aid survival. We propose an end to this monotropy, and a renewed focus in parenting research upon both male and female responsiveness to multimodal infant cues. This includes hom*osexual fathers as primary caregivers, in addition to fathers as secondary caregivers [106].

What we do know exemplifies the importance of studying behavioural data relating affective response to action. The first step in understanding gendered caregiving behaviour is to explore the motivational salience of cute infant cues. Here, results have been mixed. While women have been found to display greater overt positive appraisals of infant facial features compared to significantly lower attractiveness ratings in men (Figure 1B) [22, 107, 108]. Yet, any hedonic response involves both a ‘liking’ and a ‘wanting’ component [8], and results showing gender discrepancy have principally focused upon explicit evaluations of liking. When given a key press task conferring control over the viewing time of infant stimuli, men have shown similar incentive salience to women (Figure 1B) [22] – although see also [28]. Such findings suggest that men may be less conscious of, or less willing to admit, the compelling nature of infant cues. Furthermore, these responses can be modulated by expertise in both parents as shown by fathers exhibiting similar brain activity to mothers in regions relating to salience, reward and empathy, among others, when watching footage of themselves interacting with their infant [106]. In addition, another cross-sectional study has shown that parenthood may lead mothers to become more sensitive to infant's emotions, while fathers become less sensitive [109]. Yet, clearly the field needs longitudinal research into the development of parental responsiveness in both sexes to infant cues, combining neuroimaging and behavioural responsiveness measures.

Box 2: Cuteness computations, time and metastability

Cuteness facilitates survival which relies on time-critical neural computations [64] helping to optimize the resources used for exploration and exploitation of potential rewards ensuring long-term stability [110]. This balance between fast and slow processing is not easily struck [111] and humans are often relatively poor at temporal discounting, sometimes disastrously so [112].

Elucidating the networks underlying cuteness processing in the human brain requires more than just correlational neuroimaging. The use of whole-brain computational modelling combining structural connectomes with functional dynamics to explore and explain the emergence of resting-state and task-related networks mechanistically is starting to make significant progress in understanding the underlying brain networks for balancing fast and slow neural processing [91]. This computational modelling has started to use time-dependent activity to constrain e.g. the Hopf bifurcation model [113, 114], which captures the significant features of previously used asynchronous and oscillatory models [115]. These computational models provide evidence for the importance of metastability [116, 117], which is a measure of how variable brain states are as a function of time; e.g. how the synchronization between the different brain regions fluctuates across time. Furthermore, they have demonstrated how the healthy brain is maximally metastable, allowing for optimal exploration of the effective dynamical repertoire of patterns [92]. If the underlying structural connectivity is damaged, as found in many neuropsychiatric disorders, a much more limited functional repertoire is available, leading to potentially severe behavioural and emotional consequences [97]. As such these methods may help identify biomarkers of impaired brain processing related to cuteness which in turn may help develop new effective interventions.

Outstanding questions

  • What is the developmental trajectory of cuteness perception?

  • What are the brain mechanisms and networks linking the fast and slow cuteness responses?

  • How can cuteness best be used to improve caregiving in troubled parent-infant relationship?

  • What is the relationship between cute vocalisations and cute melodies?

  • How can we best harness cuteness to expand our moral circle and minimize dehumanisation?

Trends Box

  • * The parent-infant relationship is fundamental to infant survival and development.

  • * Cuteness has emerged as an important factor for attracting caregiver attention and affection

  • * Cuteness is not limited to visual infant features but is also found in positive sounds and smells

  • * Neuroimaging has started to identify how survival-related infant positive and negative stimuli elicits core affective brain activity through fast attentional biasing and slow appraisal processes

  • * Beyond caregiving, cuteness plays a key role in facilitating social relationships, pleasure and well-being, as well as increase empathy and compassion

    See Also
    Cuteness

Glossary

BifurcationAn abrupt qualitative change in the system's dynamics when one or more parameter pass through critical values, for instance the loss of stability and appearance of sustained oscillations.
ConnectomeThe complete description of the structural connections between elements of a nervous system.
Dynamical systemsAn area of applied mathematics that describes the behaviour of complex (possibly chaotic) dynamical systems as described by differential or difference equations.
Hopf bifurcationIn nonlinear dynamics, a Hopf bifurcation is a local bifurcation in which an initially stable fixed point of a dynamical system loses its stability in an oscillatory fashion.
Global neuronal workspace modelA model that proposes that conscious access occurs once a stimulus gains access through ignition to a global neuronal workspace, where information is broadly shared and broadcasts it to many other processors.
Magnetoencephalography (MEG)A method of measuring brain activity by detecting minute perturbations in the extracranial magnetic field that are generated by the electrical activity of neuronal populations.
MetastabilityIn dynamical systems refers to a state which falls outside the natural equilibrium state of the system but persists for an extended period of time.
MonotropyThe concept according to Bowlby that infants have an innate and inborn capacity to attach primarily to a single caregiver.
Moral circleThe circle of entities worthy of moral consideration, i.e. the in-group of entities to whom kindness is extended.
Postnatal (postpartum) depressionDepression after birth associated with impairments in parent–infant interactions, as well as longer term disruption of emotional and cognitive development of the infant.
Temporal discountingThe phenomenon whereby people typically devalue rewards as a function of the delay to their delivery.

Footnotes

Publisher's Disclaimer: This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Conflict of interest: the authors declare no conflict of interest.

References

1. Lorenz K. Die angeborenen Formen Möglicher Erfahrung. [Innate forms of potential experience]. Zeitschrift für Tierpsychologie. 1943;5:235–519. [Google Scholar]

2. Tinbergen N. On aims and methods of Ethology. Zeitschrift fur Tierpsychologie. 1963;20:410–433. [Google Scholar]

3. Burkhardt RW. Tribute to Tinbergen: Putting Niko Tinbergen's ‘Four Questions’ in Historical Context. Ethology. 2014;120:215–223. [Google Scholar]

4. Hinde RA. Animal Behaviour: A Synthesis of Ethology and Comparative Psychology. McGraw-Hill; 1970. [Google Scholar]

5. Hinde RA. Ethology: Its Nature and Relations with Other Sciences. Oxford Univ. Press; 1982. [Google Scholar]

6. Lehrman DS. A critique of Konrad Lorenz's theory of instinctive behavior. The Quarterly review of biology. 1953;28:337–363. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

7. Ledoux J. Rethinking the emotional brain. Neuron. 2012;73:653–676. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

8. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Affective neuroscience of pleasure: Reward in humans and animals. Psychopharmacology. 2008;199:457–480. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

9. Huron D. The plural pleasures of music. In Proceedings of the. 2004 Music and Music Science Conference. In: Sundberg J, Brunson W, editors. Kungliga Musikhögskolan & KTH. Royal Institute of Technology; 2005. pp. 1–13. [Google Scholar]

10. Morton ES. On the occurrence and significance of motivation-structural rules in some bird and mammal sounds. American Naturalist. 1977:855–869. [Google Scholar]

11. Darwin CR. A biographical sketch of an infant. Mind. 1877;2:286–294. [Google Scholar]

12. Sherman GD, Haidt J. Cuteness and Disgust: The Humanizing and Dehumanizing Effects of Emotion. Emot Rev. 2011;3:245–251. [Google Scholar]

13. Brosch T, et al. Beyond fear - Rapid spatial orienting toward positive emotional stimuli. Psychol Sci. 2008;19:362–370. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

14. Brosch T, et al. That baby caught my eye... attention capture by infant faces. Emotion. 2007;7:685–689. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

15. Kringelbach ML, et al. A specific and rapid neural signature for parental instinct. PLoS ONE. 2008;3:e1664. doi:1610.1371/journal.pone.0001664. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

16. Bowlby J. Attachment and Loss, Vol 1: Attachment. Hogarth Press; 1969. [Google Scholar]

17. Mayr E. Cause and effect in biology. Science. 1961;134:1501–1506. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Berry DS, Mcarthur LZ. Some Components and Consequences of a Babyface. J Pers Soc Psychol. 1985;48:312–323. [Google Scholar]

19. Hildebrandt KA, Fitzgerald HE. Adults' responses to infants varying in perceived cuteness. Behavioural Processes. 1978;3:159–172. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

20. Glocker ML, et al. Baby schema in infant faces induces cuteness perception and motivation for caretaking in adults. Ethology. 2009;115:257–263. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

21. Senese VP, et al. Human infant faces provoke implicit positive affective responses in parents and non-parents alike. PloS one. 2013;8:e80379. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

22. Parsons CE, et al. The motivational salience of infant faces is similar for men and women. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e20632. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

23. Esposito G, et al. Baby, you light-up my face: culture-general physiological responses to infants and culture-specific cognitive judgements of adults. PloS one. 2014;9:e106705. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

24. Borgi M, et al. Baby schema in human and animal faces induces cuteness perception and gaze allocation in children. Front Psychol. 2014;5 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

25. Van Duuren M, et al. Early aesthetic choices: Infant preferences for attractive premature infant faces. Int J Behav Dev. 2003;27:212–219. [Google Scholar]

26. Thompson-Booth C, et al. Here's looking at you, kid: Attention to infant emotional faces in mothers and non-mothers. Developmental Science. 2014;17:35–46. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

27. Parsons CE, et al. The Bonnie baby: experimentally manipulated temperament affects perceived cuteness and motivation to view infant faces. Developmental Science. 2013;17:257–269. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

28. Hahn AC, et al. Gender differences in the incentive salience of adult and infant faces. Q J Exp Psychol. 2013;66:200–208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

29. Golle J, et al. Preference for cute infants does not depend on their ethnicity or species: evidence from hypothetical adoption and donation paradigms. PloS one. 2015;10:e0121554. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

30. Proverbio AM, et al. Is it a baby? Perceived age affects brain processing of faces differently in women and men. J Cogn Neurosci. 2011;23:3197–3208. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

31. Riem MM, et al. No laughing matter: intranasal oxytocin administration changes functional brain connectivity during exposure to infant laughter. Neuropsychopharmacology : official publication of the American College of Neuropsychopharmacology. 2012;37:1257–1266. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

32. Seifritz E, et al. Differential sex-independent amygdala response to infant crying and laughing in parents versus nonparents. Biological psychiatry. 2003;54:1367–1375. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

33. Porter RH, et al. Maternal recognition of neonates through olfactory cues. Physiology & behavior. 1983;30:151–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

34. Papousek M. Communication in early infancy: An arena of intersubjective learning. Infant Behavior and Development. 2007;30:258–266. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

35. Parsons CE, et al. The functional neuroanatomy of the evolving parent-infant relationship. Progress in Neurobiology. 2010;91:220–241. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

36. Parsons CE, et al. Listening to infant distress vocalizations enhances effortful motor performance. Acta Paediatr. 2012;101:e189–191. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

37. Parsons CE, et al. Ready for action: A role for the brainstem in responding to infant vocalizations. Social Cognitive and Affective Neuroscience. 2014;9:977–984. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

38. Young KS, et al. Motion and emotion: depression reduces psychom*otor performance and alters affective movements in caregiving interactions. Frontiers in Behavioural neuroscience. 2015;9:26. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

39. Parsons CE, et al. Minor structural abnormalities in the infant face disrupt neural processing: a unique window into early caregiving responses. Soc Neurosci. 2013;8:268–274. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

40. Nittono H, et al. The power of Kawaii: viewing cute images promotes a careful behavior and narrows attentional focus. PloS one. 2012;7:e46362. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

41. Glocker ML, et al. Baby schema modulates the brain reward system in nulliparous women. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2009;106:9115–9119. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

42. Kringelbach ML, Rolls ET. The functional neuroanatomy of the human orbitofrontal cortex: evidence from neuroimaging and neuropsychology. Progress in Neurobiology. 2004;72:341–372. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

43. Kringelbach ML, Rapuano KM. Time in the orbitofrontal cortex. Brain. 2016 in press. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

44. Bar M, et al. Top-down facilitation of visual recognition. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2006;103:449–454. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

45. Li Y, et al. The neural dynamics of reward value and risk coding in the human orbitofrontal cortex. Brain : a journal of neurology. 2016 [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

46. Parsons CE, et al. Understanding the human parental brain: a critical role of the orbitofrontal cortex. Soc Neurosci. 2013;8:525–543. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

47. Ranote S, et al. The neural basis of maternal responsiveness to infants: an fMRI study. Neuroreport. 2004;15:1825–1829. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

48. Nitschke JB, et al. Orbitofrontal cortex tracks positive mood in mothers viewing pictures of their newborn infants. NeuroImage. 2004;21:583–592. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

49. Strathearn L, et al. What's in a smile? Maternal brain responses to infant facial cues. Pediatrics. 2008;122:40–51. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

50. Little AC. Manipulation of Infant-Like Traits Affects Perceived Cuteness of Infant, Adult and Cat Faces. Ethology. 2012;118:775–782. [Google Scholar]

51. Archer J, Monton S. Preferences for Infant Facial Features in Pet Dogs and Cats. Ethology. 2011;117:217–226. [Google Scholar]

52. Gould SJ. The Panda's Thumb: More Reflections in Natural History. W.W. Norton & Company; 1980. A Biological Homage to Mickey Mouse. [Google Scholar]

53. Hinde RA, Barden LA. The evolution of the teddy bear. Animal Behaviour. 1985;33:1371–1373. [Google Scholar]

54. Bornstein MH. Human Infancy...and the Rest of the Lifespan. Annual Review of Psychology. 2014;65:121–158. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

55. Konner M. The Evolution of Childhood: Relationships, Emotion, Mind. Harvard University Press; 2010. [Google Scholar]

56. Barrett J, Fleming AS. Annual Research Review: All mothers are not created equal: neural and psychobiological perspectives on mothering and the importance of individual differences. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2011;52:368–397. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

57. Feldman R. The adaptive human parental brain: implications for children's social development. Trends in neurosciences. 2015;38:387–399. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

58. Young KS, et al. Evidence for a caregiving instinct: rapid differentiation of infant from adult vocalisations using magnetoencephalography. Cereb Cortex. 2016;26:1309–1321. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

59. Bornstein MH, et al. Emotional Relationships in Mothers and Infants: Culture-Common and Community-Specific Characteristics of Dyads from Rural and Metropolitan Settings in Argentina, Italy, and the United States. J Cross Cult Psychol. 2012;43:171–197. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

60. Kim P, et al. Neural plasticity in fathers of human infants. Soc Neurosci. 2014;9:522–535. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

61. Kim P, et al. The plasticity of human maternal brain: longitudinal changes in brain anatomy during the early postpartum period. Behavioral neuroscience. 2010;124:695–700. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

62. Swain JE. The human parental brain: in vivo neuroimaging. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry. 2011;35:1242–1254. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

63. Kringelbach ML, Phillips H. Emotion: pleasure and pain in the brain. Oxford University Press; 2014. [Google Scholar]

64. Berridge KC, Kringelbach ML. Pleasure systems in the brain. Neuron. 2015;86:646–664. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

65. Riem MM, et al. Attachment in the brain: adult attachment representations predict amygdala and behavioral responses to infant crying. Attach Hum Dev. 2012;14:533–551. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

66. Doi H, Shinohara K. The perceived duration of emotional face is influenced by the gaze direction. Neuroscience letters. 2009;457:97–100. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

67. Noriuchi M, et al. The Functional Neuroanatomy of Maternal Love: Mother's Response to Infant's Attachment Behaviors. Biological psychiatry. 2008;63:415–423. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

68. Bartels A, Zeki S. The neural correlates of maternal and romantic love. NeuroImage. 2004;21:1155–1166. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

69. Spindler P. Studien zur vererbung von verhaltensweisen. 3. Verhalten gegenuber jungen katzen [Studies on the transmission of behavioral patterns. 3. Behavior toward young cats]. Anthropologischer Anzeiger. 1961;25:60–80. [Google Scholar]

70. Heckman JJ. Skill formation and the economics of investing in disadvantaged children. Science. 2006;312:1900–1902. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

71. O'Hara MW, Swain AM. Rates and risk of postpartum depression - a meta-analysis. International Review of Psychiatry. 1996;8:37–54. [Google Scholar]

72. Paulson JF, Bazemore SD. Prenatal and postpartum depression in fathers and its association with maternal depression: a meta-analysis. Jama. 2010;303:1961–1969. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

73. Parsons CE, et al. Postnatal depression and its effects on child development: a review of evidence from low- and middle-income countries. Br Med Bull. 2012;101:57–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

74. Bigelow AE, et al. Maternal sensitivity throughout infancy: Continuity and relation to attachment security. Infant Behavior and Development. 2010;33:50–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

75. Manian N, Bornstein MH. Dynamics of emotion regulation in infants of clinically depressed and nondepressed mothers. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2009;50:1410–1418. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

76. Lester BM, et al. Developmental outcome as a function of the goodness of fit between the infant's cry characteristics and the mother's perception of her infant's cry. Pediatrics. 1995;95:516–521. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

77. Donovan WL, et al. Conflict and depression predict maternal sensitivity to infant cries. Infant Behavior and Development. 1998;21:505–517. [Google Scholar]

78. Stein A, et al. Interpretation of infant facial expression in the context of maternal postnatal depression. Infant Behav Dev. 2010;33:273–278. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

79. Young KS, et al. Interpreting Infant Vocal Distress: The Ameliorative Effect of Musical Training in Depression. Emotion. 2012;12:1200–1205. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

80. van Ijzendoorn MH, et al. Disorganized attachment in early childhood: meta-analysis of precursors, concomitants, and sequelae. Developmental Psychopathology. 1999;11:225–249. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

81. Pearson RM, et al. Maternal perinatal mental health and offspring academic achievement at age 16: the mediating role of childhood executive function. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2015 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

82. Goodacre T, Swan MC. Cleft lip and palate: current management. Paediatrics and Child Health. 2008;18:283–292. [Google Scholar]

83. Murray L, et al. The effect of cleft lip and palate, and the timing of lip repair on mother-infant interactions and infant development. Journal of child psychology and psychiatry, and allied disciplines. 2008;49:115–123. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

84. Parsons CE, et al. The effect of cleft lip on adults’ responses to faces: cross-species findings. PLoS ONE. 2011;6:e25897. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

85. Bhandari R, et al. Effects of intranasal oxytocin administration on memory for infant cues: moderation by childhood emotional maltreatment. Soc Neurosci. 2014;9:536–547. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

86. Parsons CE, et al. Music training and empathy positively impact adults' sensitivity to infant distress. Front Psychol. 2014;5:1440. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

87. Barrett LF, et al. The experience of emotion. Annu Rev Psychol. 2007;58:373–403. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

88. Frijda NH. The emotions. Cambridge University Press; 1986. [Google Scholar]

89. Kringelbach ML, Berridge KC. Towards a functional neuroanatomy of pleasure and happiness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences. 2009;13:479–487. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

90. Rømer Thomsen K, et al. Reconceptualising anhedonia: novel perspectives on balancing the pleasure networks in the human brain. Frontiers in Behavioural Neuroscience. 2015;9:49. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

91. Kringelbach ML, et al. The rediscovery of slowness: exploring the timing of cognition. TICS. 2015;19:616–628. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

92. Deco G, Kringelbach ML. Metastability and Coherence: Extending the Communication through Coherence Hypothesis Using A Whole-Brain Computational Perspective. Trends in neurosciences. 2016;39:125–135. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

93. Mesulam MM. From sensation to cognition. Brain : a journal of neurology. 1998;121:1013–1052. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

94. Baars BJ. A Cognitive Theory of Consciousness. Cambridge University Press; 1989. [Google Scholar]

95. Dehaene S, et al. A neuronal model of a global workspace in effortful cognitive tasks. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 1998;95:14529–14534. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

96. Lagercrantz H, Changeux JP. The emergence of human consciousness: from fetal to neonatal life. Pediatr Res. 2009;65:255–260. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

97. Deco G, Kringelbach ML. Great Expectations: Using Whole-Brain Computational Connectomics for Understanding Neuropsychiatric Disorders. Neuron. 2014;84:892–905. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

98. Deco G, et al. Rethinking segregation and integration: contributions of whole-brain modelling. Nat Rev Neurosci. 2015;16:430–439. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

99. Singer P. The expanding circle: Ethics and sociobiology. Farrar, Straus, & Giroux. 1981 [Google Scholar]

100. Rozin P, Fallon AE. A perspective on disgust. Psychol Rev. 1987;94:23–41. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

101. Young KS, et al. Frühholz S, Belin P, editors. Neural responses to infant vocalisations in adult listeners. The Oxford Handbook of Voice Perception. 2016 in press. [Google Scholar]

102. Kringelbach ML. The orbitofrontal cortex: linking reward to hedonic experience. Nature Reviews Neuroscience. 2005;6:691–702. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

103. Dehaene S, Changeux JP. Experimental and theoretical approaches to conscious processing. Neuron. 2011;70:200–227. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

104. Chanes L, Barrett LF. Redefining the Role of Limbic Areas in Cortical Processing. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2016;20:96–106. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

105. Solantaus T, Salo S. Paternal postnatal depression: fathers emerge from the wings. Lancet. 2005;365:2158–2159. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

106. Abraham E, et al. Father's brain is sensitive to childcare experiences. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America. 2014;111:9792–9797. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

107. Sprengelmeyer R, et al. The cutest little baby face: A hormonal link to sensitivity to cuteness in infant faces. Psychol Sci. 2009;20:149–154. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

108. Lobmaier JS, et al. Female and male responses to cuteness, age and emotion in infant faces. Evolution and Human Behavior. 2010;31:16–21. [Google Scholar]

109. Parsons CE, et al. Interpreting infant emotional expressions: parenthood has differential effects on men and women. Quarterly journal of experimental psychology. 2016:1–19. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

110. Kringelbach ML, et al. Balancing the brain: resting state networks and deep brain stimulation. Frontiers Integrative Neuroscience. 2011;5:8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

111. Kahneman D. Thinking, fast and slow. Farrar, Straus & Giroux. 2011 [Google Scholar]

112. Peters J, Buchel C. The neural mechanisms of inter-temporal decision-making: understanding variability. Trends in cognitive sciences. 2011;15:227–239. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

113. Freyer F, et al. Biophysical mechanisms of multistability in resting-state cortical rhythms. The Journal of neuroscience : the official journal of the Society for Neuroscience. 2011;31:6353–6361. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

114. Freyer F, et al. A canonical model of multistability and scale-invariance in biological systems. PLoS Comput Biol. 2012;8:e1002634. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

115. Cabral J, et al. Exploring the network dynamics underlying brain activity during rest. Prog Neurobiol. 2014;114:102–131. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

116. Haken H. Cooperative phenomena in systems far from thermal equilibrium and in nonphysical systems. Reviews of Modern Physics. 1975;47:67–121. [Google Scholar]

117. Tognoli E, Kelso JA. The metastable brain. Neuron. 2014;81:35–48. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

On cuteness: unlocking the parental brain and beyond (2024)

FAQs

Is cuteness in offspring a potent protective PTE? ›

Cuteness in offspring is a potent protective mechanism that ensures survival for otherwise completely dependent infants.

What is the physiology of cuteness? ›

Cuteness is not limited to visual infant features, but is also found in positive sounds and smells. Neuroimaging has started to identify how survival-related infant-positive and negative stimuli elicit core affective brain activity through fast attentional biasing and slow appraisal processes.

Are babies cute for survival? ›

Babies need constant attention and care to survive, and cuteness is one of the main ways they get it.

Is being cute a survival strategy? ›

Your puppy's cuteness isn't just a thing to be admired. It may very well be an evolutionary trait that helps them survive and grow up to become healthy adult dogs. In fact, many babies of mammals have cute traits that help them survive. Here's what we know about how dogs have evolved to become cute in our eyes.

Is cuteness an adaptation? ›

In fact, just glimpsing at it will trigger an innate caregiving mechanism, a neurological response that's been sharpened across thousands of years of human evolution. “Ultimately, this cuteness response is an important adaptation for us.

What are the two types of cuteness? ›

In fact, cute can be separated into two categories: baby cuteness and whimsical cuteness. Our brains react differently to the two distinct types of cuteness, a differentiation that marketers have begun to pay attention to.

What is the psychology behind cuteness? ›

Psychologically speaking, the reason individuals are drawn to cute things is that they make them feel protective, caring, and affectionate. Our brains, experience a pleasant emotional reaction when we view something charming, such an adorable baby animal or a cartoon character with large eyes and little features.

Does cuteness release dopamine? ›

"Dopamine is one of the most important hormones that trigger happiness and a positive emotional response," Sehat says. "Whenever we see tiny things we find cute and attractive, our brain releases dopamine and makes us feel happy."

Is there an evolutionary benefit to cuteness? ›

Although cuteness can be a matter of personal preference, people tend to find young mammals cute and adorable, and this is likely part of an evolutionary development. Cuteness serves a purpose in species whose young cannot live on their own right away.

What is the cute baby theory? ›

Lorenz proposed the concept of baby schema (Kindchenschema), a set of facial and body features that make a creature appear "cute" and activate ("release") in others the motivation to care for it. Cuteness may be ascribed to people as well as things that are regarded as attractive or charming.

At what age are babies most cute? ›

In fact, the results of a recent survey published in Evolution and Human Behavior found that we don't find babies cute until three, or even six months of age. 1 From there, babies remain at peak cuteness until around age four-and-a-half. Understandable right? That's when they're at their most chubby and dimpled.

What is the evolutionary purpose of cuteness? ›

Konrad Lorenz argued in 1949 that infantile features triggered nurturing responses in adults and that this was an evolutionary adaptation which helped ensure that adults cared for their children, ultimately securing the survival of the species.

What is the concept of cuteness? ›

The modern concept of cuteness has been influenced by the work of an Austrian ethologist, Konrad Lorenz, who coined the term Kindchenschema (baby schema) to refer to child-like features such as small body size with a large head, large eyes, round cheeks, small limbs, plump body shape, soft body surface, as well as ...

What is the science behind squeezing cute things? ›

Biological response of oxytocin attaches the adults to infants and vasopressin is somehow associated with aggressive feelings. Cute aggression, such as biting, squeezing, and tackling, is related to the intersection of emotional responses and reward centers.

Can our brains comprehend cuteness? ›

Neurological Response: When humans encounter cuteness, the brain's reward system is activated. Neurotransmitters like dopamine, associated with pleasure and satisfaction, are released. This reaction is not just limited to human babies but extends to animals and even inanimate objects exhibiting baby-like features.

Top Articles
Michael A. Einhorn, Ph.D. 973-618-1212€¦ · 2 Michael A. Einhorn, Ph.D. 973-618-1212 MEDIA, TECHNOLOGY, COPYRIGHT Video: Movies (Paramount/Dreamworks), cable programs (NBCUniversal), - [PDF Document]
Vasicek Interest Rate Model - Overview, Formula, Applications
Suppression du CESE et du HCCT au Sénégal : L'Assemblée nationale vote contre la suppression de ces deux institutions - BBC News Afrique
Order Irs Tax Forms Online
The Meaning Behind The Song: Waymore's Blues by Waylon Jennings - Beat Crave
Midlands Tech Beltline Campus Bookstore
Crystal Lust Wiki
DRAGON BALL Z - Goku Evolution - Light Canvas 40X3 NEU • EUR 37,63
Spicy Korean Gochujang Tofu (Vegan)
Circloo Unblocked
Ttw Cut Content
Lorton Transfer Station
Dmv Leestown Rd
Praxis für Psychotherapie und Coaching Rhein-Neckar
Leaf Blower and Vacuum Vacuum Hoses
Craigslist Chester Sc
Myworld Interactive American History Pdf
Norte Asesores Nanda
2Lookmovie
How to Be an Extra in a Movie (and What to Expect)
Holly Ranch Aussie Farm
Walmart Supercenter Nearest To My Location
Uganda: The tiny flea making it painful for people to walk and work | African Arguments
Venus Nail Lounge Lake Elsinore
Roomba I3 Sealing Problem With Clean Base
Antique Wedding Favors
Www.citizen-Times.com Obituaries
SimpliSafe Home Security Review: Still a Top DIY Choice
Biopark Prices
Hux Lipford Funeral
Odu Csnbbs
Official Klj
Embu village mines precious coltan for years 'without knowing its value’
Pipa Mountain Hot Pot渝味晓宇重庆老火锅 Menu
Mellow Mushroom Nutrition Facts: What to Order & Avoid
Get Over It Stables
Sa 0 Spn 2659 Fmi 18
Seatgeek Seat View
Best Th13 Base
Hyb Urban Dictionary
North Haven Power School
Rabbi Raps
Arre St Wv Srj
Bella Poarch Husband: A Deep Dive Into Her Relationship And Personal Life
Sona Systems Tcu
Registrar Lls
Rush Copley Swim Lessons
Jersey Mike's Subs: 16 Facts About The Sandwich Chain - The Daily Meal
Understanding DeFi The Roles, Tools, Risks, and Rewards of -- Alexandra Damsker -- 2024 -- O'Reilly Media -- 9781098120764 -- 79accdb00af9d0f41d97f44fa7970ff1 -- Annas Archive - Biblioteconomia
The Enchanted Library - FiMFetch.net
Craigslist Boats Rochester
Cardaras Logan Ohio
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Wyatt Volkman LLD

Last Updated:

Views: 6310

Rating: 4.6 / 5 (46 voted)

Reviews: 85% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Wyatt Volkman LLD

Birthday: 1992-02-16

Address: Suite 851 78549 Lubowitz Well, Wardside, TX 98080-8615

Phone: +67618977178100

Job: Manufacturing Director

Hobby: Running, Mountaineering, Inline skating, Writing, Baton twirling, Computer programming, Stone skipping

Introduction: My name is Wyatt Volkman LLD, I am a handsome, rich, comfortable, lively, zealous, graceful, gifted person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.